Conversation is at the heart of learning. This is about learners (children, students) talking their way into learning.
Monday, November 13, 2017
Friday, November 10, 2017
Language is important and a pathway to reading.
I AM coming back to this blog with an emphasis on early (k-3) reading skills. There continue to be way too many 3rd graders who are not reading at 3rd grade level.
We need to examine the tools we have to teach early reading skills and why teachers and even special educators or Level 2 RTI teachers are NOT using these tools.
I continue to look for answers and suggesting helpful resource links.
Today I want to recommend a book I'm currently reading: Unlocking Literacy: Effective Decoding and Spelling Instructions (2010) by Marcia K. Henry, who has worked in the field of reading instruction for struggling readers for more than 50 years. This book is recommended by G. Reid Lyon, Louisa Moats, Virginia Berninger, Donald Deshler, Keith Stanovich, Susan Brady and Cheryl Gabig.
We need to examine the tools we have to teach early reading skills and why teachers and even special educators or Level 2 RTI teachers are NOT using these tools.
I continue to look for answers and suggesting helpful resource links.
Today I want to recommend a book I'm currently reading: Unlocking Literacy: Effective Decoding and Spelling Instructions (2010) by Marcia K. Henry, who has worked in the field of reading instruction for struggling readers for more than 50 years. This book is recommended by G. Reid Lyon, Louisa Moats, Virginia Berninger, Donald Deshler, Keith Stanovich, Susan Brady and Cheryl Gabig.
Tuesday, October 17, 2017
Becoming a Reader
How many children are learning to read in the early grades. Here is a success story.
Reading program expands to
Park City elementary schools
Teachers are trained to
help all children read, including those with dyslexia Carolyn
Webber October
13, 2017
Reading program expands to
Park City elementary schools
Teachers are trained to
help all children read, including those with dyslexia Carolyn
Webber October
13, 2017
Thursday, July 6, 2017
From Discourse to Reading Comprehension?
When should we start addressing reading comprehension? Is Kindergarten too early to work on reading
comprehension skills?
We know that parents and other adults including preschool
teachers are encouraged to begin engaging babies and toddlers in book “reading”. Of course, the reading is being done by the
parent/teacher/adult. We also know that
adults engage children in conversations about books, using, for example, “Dialogic
Reading”.
When might we begin to “teach” reading comprehension skills
in a somewhat more formal sense?
Let’s assume that it is possible to teach some reading comprehension
skills in Kindergarten. In my grandson’s
kindergarten this past year, his teacher actively engaged the class in learning
some basic reading comprehension skills—story characters, setting, and story plot. They read both narrative and information
texts. They had “favorite” books. They shared what a book was “about.” They also wrote “stories” from their personal
experience.
But are there curricula for teaching comprehension in the
primary grades. That’s what I’ll explore
in the next series of posts.
Monday, June 19, 2017
Normal Language Development and the Role of Oral Language in Common Disabilities
This is a companion piece to the last posting on Language Development. Children with disabilities like Autism, Learning Disabilities, and Intellectual Disabilities are very likely to have delayed oral language or an oral language disability. Here is a power point explaining the areas that a speech/language/ pathologist might address.
What role does Oral Language Play in “Disabilities”
Speech-Language Pathologists— Who Are We, and What Do We Do
From the American Speech Language and Hearing Association,
May, 2017
From ASHA Access Schools
Monday, May 22, 2017
The Role of Oral Language in Literacy and Learning
The role of Oral Language in Literacy and Learning
http://92Y.org/WonderplayConference | With anecdotes from her own vast
professional experience as a language Pathologist, Dr. Soifer discussed how
Oral language is the foundation of literacy and a crucial skill for learning
and social development. Oral language develops much faster than many people
realize. Good language skills have far reaching influences on children's
development. The specifics of language
Published on Nov 20, 2012
1 hour 20 minutes
This is long but well worth your time. It is like a mini course in Language Development.
Labels:
Achievement Gap,
Conversation,
Early Literacy,
Oral Language
Monday, May 1, 2017
Sentence Functions and Conversation: The heart of language development
SENTENCE
FUNCTIONS
7 Functions of Language expressed at the sentence level by Michael Halliday
7 Functions of Language expressed at the sentence level by Michael Halliday
Instrumental: I
want a banana
Regulatory:
“First I … you need a rake and you have to build over the rake.”
Interactional:
“Do you like cricket too Henry?”
Personal:
“I know that song ‘cause we sang it at Kindergarten.”
Heuristic:
“We could make a water thing to tell how much rain we got.”
Imaginative:
“Alice the camel has one hump, one hump, one hump.”
Representational:
“It is raining really heavy and heavy all day.”
From: Michael
Halliday’s chapter in Language Development:
A Reader for Teachers edited by Brenda Miller Power and Ruth S. Hubbard,
Merrill Prentice Hall, 2002
The chapter
begins with a question asked by a teacher of English: “What is language?”, to
which Halliday responds: “Why do you
want to know?” Halliday’s point is that it matters why you want/need to
define language. He takes the perspective
of the child learning language: that is, the child’s “Model” of language
internalized as a result of his experience. “The child knows what language is
because he knows what language does.” [That is, the function(s) of language].
Halliday goes on to write:
“….The
determining elements of the young child’s experience are the successful demands
on language that he himself has made, the particular needs that have been
satisfied by language for him….”
“…We shall try
to identify the models of language with which the normal child is endowed by
the time he comes to school at the age of 5….”
Halliday then
goes on to describe “models” of language function the child brings to school:
Instrumental, Regulatory, Interactional, Personal, Heuristic, Imaginative, and
Representational. See the chart above for the categories, the functions they
are designed to achieve, and examples.
Language Use
and Learning
In the same
text on Language Development: A Reader for Teachers, Gay Su Pinnell describes
“Ways to Look at the Functions” of Children’s Language”* in which she offers
examples based on classroom observations, a process for doing those
observations, and ways to enhance children’s 7 functional uses of language.
*Taken from
Observing the Language Learner” (pp. 57-72), A. Jaggar and M. T. Smith-Burke
(Eds., Newark, DE: International Reading Association, 1985
There are many
texts and example that address the relationship between oral language and
school success, some of which we will refer to as to move to our next level of
oral language—discourse. Many of these are from the 1970’s beginning with a
seminal work by Cazden and Hymes in 1972 (Functions of Language in the
Classroom). The date is mentioned not to suggest that these sources, ideas, and
concerns are outdated; but, rather, to suggest that language use and school
success is an old and ongoing topic of interest and importance. More recent
texts include:
******
The Functions
of Language (at the sentence level): Some examples and excerpts from Halliday.
So, when a
child uses a sentence we need to pay attention to its function, not just its
form.. Note that these examples are at the sentence level, but they only
take on meaning as they function as part of a discourse/conversation….both oral
and written.
Instrumental: Language is used as a means of
getting things done.
I want… a car, boat, treat…. I want to…go home, find a book…
I want… a car, boat, treat…. I want to…go home, find a book…
“Success in
this use of language does not in any way depend on the production of
well-formed adult sentences; a carefully contextualized yell may have
substantially the same effect…”
Regulatory: Language is used to regulate the
behavior of others.
You
shouldn’t….tear the paper,
go there; use that pencil, take that book..
go there; use that pencil, take that book..
While this function of language, in the
child’s experience, may initially be used by adults, the child learns to use
it, too.
Interactional: Language is used to address social
interactions—both personal and group interactions.
Let’s work on this together.
We can do this.
You don’t belong here.
Let’s work on this together.
We can do this.
You don’t belong here.
Personal: Language is used reflect self,
his/her personality, uniqueness
I’m good at math.
I like to play dominoes.
I think school is fun.
I’m good at math.
I like to play dominoes.
I think school is fun.
Heuristic. Language used to explore one’s
environment—language as a means of investigating reality, a way of learning about
things.
I have a question?
Do that mean…?
Oh, look, there’s a frog.
I have a question?
Do that mean…?
Oh, look, there’s a frog.
“Imaginative.
Language used to create his/her own “environment” as he/she can image it to be.
Hi, Mr. Pepperoni Pants.
This is a zigo.
The request of the buggy coming right up. Humming…
Once there was a big tree house…
Hi, Mr. Pepperoni Pants.
This is a zigo.
The request of the buggy coming right up. Humming…
Once there was a big tree house…
Representational: Language
used to communicate about something, for expressing propositions.
Mars is a
million miles away.
The zoo has many wild animals.
Molly is sick.
The zoo has many wild animals.
Molly is sick.
* * * *
More example as
we move to the next section on oral language development: The Discourse or
Conversation Level.
Labels:
Conversation,
Early Literacy,
Oral Language,
Sentences
Monday, April 24, 2017
Processing Complex Sentences Can Impact Speaking, Listening, and Reading Development
“Sentence” is the third unit (sound-word-sentence-discourse)
of oral language described as a primary basis for learning to read and reading
to learn. Developmentally, the “average”
child begins to put words together to form “sentences” by age 2. They don’t know the “parts of speech” names, but they do learn that there are
certain kinds of words that go together (nouns and verbs, nouns and adjectives,
verbs and adverbs) to achieve a certain purpose. As they continue to develop, their sentences
grow longer and more complex.
I write about sentences in terms of three dimensions: structure, process(ing) and function. I began with structure because structure
(form/grammar) is often the way sentences are talked about by adults and are addressed
in school curriculum.
Sentence Processing
There is more to
sentence development than its form or structure (syntax). Equally important for
the development of literacy (reading) skills are processing of sentences and
using sentences for communication—function.
Processing complex
sentences
From a developmental perspective Loban’s research with 211
students followed from K to grade 12 shows the following about students’ skills
at processing language at the sentence level:
*Those superior in oral language in kindergarten and grade
one before they learn to read and write are the very ones who excel in
reading and writing by the time they are in grade 6.
^In sentences in oral language, the high group in the 1st
grade demonstrates a proficiency in elaboration
attained by the low group only at grades 5 or 6.
^Although all subjects knew and used all the basic
structural patterns of English sentence, the high group had a much greater flexibility and repertoire within
the pattern of a sentence: that is, they had more ways to fill slots like
the subject, the modifiers, and the objects.
^The members of the high group used more subordination, combining communication units in complex fashion
^The high group showed
*longer communication units
*greater elaboration of subject and predicate
*more embedding in transformational grammar
*greater
use of adjectival dependent clauses
Difficulties in attending to or producing long and/or
complex sentences impacts reading in a number of ways.
For example, in her text on Teaching Reading Comprehension Processes (1991), Judith Westphal
Irwin describes three kinds of comprehension skills: metaprocesses,
macroprocesses and microprocesses. Under
microprocesses, Irwin describes the importance of chunking words into
meaningful phrases, selecting what is most important from the sentence to keep
in short term memory for interpretation of upcoming sentences (that is,
microselection); and then using that information across sentence boundaries
(integrative processes).
“Integrative
processes” she defined as the processes involved in understanding and inferring
the relationships between clauses and sentences. Three main types of integrative processes are
understanding anaphora,
understanding connective relationships,
and making “slot filling” inference.”
(Irwin, page 38). In the following
pages, she offers several suggestions for teaching these skills. (pp. 38-49)
* *
*
Cheryl M. Scott, Chapter 16: Syntax in Stone, et. al.
(Eds.), Handbook of Language and Literacy:
Development and Disorders, (2005)
Scott lists three things that make sentences complex:
1)
Features of open class words (nouns, verbs); reversible
passive sentences
2)
Number and type of syntactic operations (usually reflected in sentence length)
3)
Types of syntactic operations (reversible order,
subordination, ambiguity, parsing, etc.)
So, just as is the case in Words/Vocabulary, the complexity
of sentence and the operations required to process them, (anaphora,
connectives, inferences, for example) influences how easily a child can process
sentences.
This reminds us of the quote from the work of Amanda C. Brandone, Sara J. Salkind, Roberta Michnick Golinkoff, U. of
Delaware and Kathy Hirsh-Pasek:
“… Although most grammatical structures are
in place by the age of 5, children continue to acquire more complex forms and
rules of grammar in the school setting….”
Although the “average” child has mastered the basic structures
of sentences by age 5, children still need to learn the more complex structures
that will require more demanding processing.
The question we need to ask is how they will learn these sentence
structures in order to process them efficiently and effectively. This does not mean that we should focus on
directly teaching “grammar” to young children as a basis for reading. Young children (preschool through primary grades)
learn language skill through meaningful contexts. Sentences become meaningful to children when
we focus on the “functions” of sentences, part 3 of our “sentence” topic.
* *
*
Wednesday, April 19, 2017
Let's Try "Sentence" Structure Again
From the original blogpost about Sentnences - Sentence Structure
"Dialogue About Language, Literacy, and Learning" Blog for April 17, 2014 Part 1 of 3
Sentences (Part 1 of 3)
“Sentence” is the next unit (Sound-Word-Sentence-Discourse) of
oral language that provides a basis for learning to read and reading to
learn. Developmentally, the “average”
child begins to put words together to form “sentences” by age 2. What do children know about sentences as they
begin to put them together? They don’t
know the names of the parts of
speech that make up sentences (nouns, verbs, adverbs, etc., or the role of
those in sentences as subjects, objects…etc.), but they do learn that there are
certain kinds of words that go together (nouns and verbs, nouns and adjectives,
verbs and adverbs). As they continue to
develop, their sentences grow longer and more complex.
I am going to write about sentences in terms of three
dimensions: structure, process(ing) and
function. I’m going to begin with
structure because structure (form/grammar) is often the way sentences are
approached in school.
STRUCTURE
A few
sites are helpful. First is a chapter on
Language Development by Amanda C. Brandone, Sara J. Salkind, Roberta
Michnick Golinkoff, U. of Delaware and Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Temple
University
On Language
Development. (It was not clear from
the link which book contained this chapter)
http://udel.edu/~roberta/pdfs/Bear%20chaptBrandone.pdf In this chapter, the authors use content,
form, and use as their framework for discussing language development. They write:
“To better address typical and
atypical language development as well as strategies of prevention and
intervention, the five structural components of language-- phonology, semantics, syntax, morphology, and pragmatics--. may be simplified into three essential
aspects of communication: content, form, and use (Bloom & Lahey, 1978). Content refers to the semantics of
language—the concepts and ideas that are encoded in words. Form is the way in which meaning
is represented, including speech, sign language, and writing. In the context of
spoken language, form encompasses phonology, morphology, and syntax. Finally, use refers to the function of
language in context. Although each of these aspects of language can be
identified separately, they are inherently interconnected elements in
communication (Bloom & Lahey). Language problems may arise when there is a
disruption within anyone component of the model or in their integration. The
following sections consider the typical development of each of these aspects of
language.”
Language Development
This link shows overall language development, but there are
several references to the development of sentences:
“The next crucial milestone in the development of language
form occurs when the child discovers that rule-based combinations of words
actually express more than the meaning of any of the individual words. For
example,
by 17 months children are able to discriminate between
‘‘Cookie Monster is tickling Big Bird’’ and ‘‘Big Bird is tickling Cookie
Monster’’ (Hirsh-Pasek &
Golinkoff,1996). Comprehension of these rule-based combinations comes prior to
production using these rules. Children begin to combine words into two-word
utterances (e.g., ‘‘car go’’ and ‘‘more juice’’) between 18 and 24 months.
These early word combinations express meaningful relationships yet tend to be
missing function words (the, a), auxiliary verbs (am, is, has), and the bound
morphemes that mark plural (s), possessive (-’s), or tense (-ing,-ed). As
children learn to combine words into longer sequences, they add the function
words and bound morphemes that were absent from their first…”
Their Milestone charts show
language development of content, form, and use.
Note here the reference to syntax/sentence development across charts.
12-18 mos.
Semantic roles are expressed in one-word speech,
including agent, action, object, location, possession, rejection,
disappearance, nonexistence, denial.
Words are understood outside of routine games; still
need contextual support for lexical comprehension.
18–24 mos.
Average expressive vocabulary size: 200–300 words at 24
mos.
Prevalent relations expressed: agent–action,
agent–object, action–object, action–location, entity–location,
possessor–possession, demonstrative–entity, attribute-entity.
^Understands basic semantic roles and relations; two
word utterances and two syllable words emerge
^Utterances are telegraphic with few grammatical
markers
24–30 mos.
Understanding and use of questions about objects
(What?), people (Who?), and basic events (What is x doing? Where is x going?
^Use of no, not, don’t as negation between subject and
verb; Use of sentences with semi-auxiliaries—gonna, wanna).
30–36 mos.
Use and understand Why? questions.
36–42 mos.
Use and understand semantic relationship between
adjacent and conjoined sentences, including additive, temporal, causal,
contrastive.
^Present tense auxiliaries appear; be verbs used
inconsistently
42–48 mos.
Use and understand ‘‘when’’ and ‘‘how’’ questions.
Use conjunctions and as well as because to conjoin
sentences.
^Early emerging complex sentences types, including full
prepositional clauses, hi clauses, simple infinitives
48–60 mos.
Use conjunctions when, so, because ,and if
“… Although most grammatical structures are in place by
the age of 5, children continue to acquire more complex forms and rules of
grammar in the school setting….”
…” For
most children, the development of language proceeds without difficulty. By the age of 5, typically developing children
have mastered the building blocks of the system and are left only to refine and
integrate their skills in order to use language in an increasingly complex
range of tasks. During the course of the development of language, there is a tremendous range of what
can be considered normal.” [Use of bold print mine.]
…” Through interaction with family, peers, teachers, and
caregivers, children learn communicative competence, or how to use language
appropriately and strategically in social situations (Hymes, 1967). Because we
use language for so many purposes, many skills are involved in communicative
competence (see Becker-Bryant, 2001)…”
“Children need
to learn to ask questions, make requests, give orders, express agreement or
disagreement, apologize, refuse, joke, praise, and tell stories. They must
learn social routines (such as saying ‘‘Trick or treat’’ on
Halloween), terms of politeness, and ways to address
others. Children must also understand
how to initiate, maintain, and conclude conversations, as well as take turns,
provide and respond effectively to feedback, and stay on-topic. Crucially, they
must learn to be sensitive to their audience and to the situations in which
they are communicating. Sophistication in pragmatics continues to develop
throughout childhood and into adulthood. [bold mine]
Some additional references
for sentence form/syntax:
Some resources posted on the Brandone et. al.
link:
The public page of the website for the American
Speech-Language-Hearing Association offers resources to help all audiences
better understand communication and communication disorders. It also provides
links to early intervention references and professional referral services for
access to qualified care.
The Bamford-Lahey Children’s Foundation is a foundation
dedicated to conducting and supporting programs that will enhance the
linguistic, cognitive, social, and emotional development of children. The
Foundation’s current focus is improving the language development of children
with language difficulties.
This website provides a thorough list of references on
language disorders as well as information relevant to the goal of developing
guidelines on evidence-based practices in child language disorders
And two other google search links:
Language Development; short; commercial
Images for syntax development
Part 2 of the Sentence Series is on sentence processing.
Labels:
Achievement Gap,
Early Literacy,
Oral Language,
Sentences
Saturday, April 8, 2017
Vocabulary Development: Part 5 Instruction Age 3 to Grade 3
Vocabulary Instruction begins early in the form of parent or
adult-child conversation, as noted in the last posting. There is also more “intentional ” vocabulary
instruction at the preschool-primary grade levels. *
I would start by suggesting a few sources:
Speaking and Listening for Preschool Through Third Grade by
Lauren B. Resnick and Catherine B. Snow, published in 2008 by the International
Reading Association. In this booklet
(with video) they list 3 strands of Standards: (1) Habits, (2) Kinds of Talk
and Resulting Genres, and (3) Language Use and Conventions. The focus on vocabulary happens under
Standard 3.3: Vocabulary and Word Choice.
Another resources: (*I haven't tried to copy a slideshare link, so I'm not sure how well it will show.)
Please note that this was a 2 hour workshop with time for interacting with the materials/handouts.
Handouts from this Workshop.
For complete handout, contact
Toomeyand12@gmail.com
Powerful
Vocabulary, NAEYC Conference, October 2016, Fran Toomey (toomeyand12@gmai.com Handout 1 – Frameworks (CHOOSE
ONE)
Thirty Million Words
Dana Suskind, M.D., 0-3 Years
|
Bridging the Gap, Christ & Wang
(Young Children, 2010)
|
Beginning Literacy with Language, (age
3-5+) Dickinson & Tabors
|
Speaking & Listening for Preschool
Through 3rd Grade Resnick & Snow
|
Toomey Synthesis
|
Tune In: Follow the child’s lead: notice what child is doing
and join in, using “motherese,” responding
to child’s response (behavioral and verbal), use repetition (same
stories, same words), get on the same physical level.
|
Provide
purposeful exposure to new words
*Teach thematically to provide multiple
exposures to words throughout the day through read-alouds, conversation,
centers and projects. *Select books in which illustrations and text provide
clues to word meanings
*Use interactive read-aloud style to
engage children in cognitively challenging talk about books
*Create media center where children
view DVDs, electronic books and
interactive read alouds
|
Conversation
& Dialogic Reading
In the Home--Book Reading: Immediate Talk (about the book) and
Non-immediate Talk (recollection of personal experiences, comments/qs about
general knowledge (P. 49)
*Choose a variety of book types
*Discuss some aspect of the book before
and after reading
*Use Intonation, gestures and point to
illustration … without interrupting flow of text.
*Make the book experience overflow into
other areas of life.
|
Domains
^Add words to familiar knowledge
domains (KD)(people, animals, foods, households)
^Sort relationships among words in KD
^Add new domains from subjects and
topics being studied.
|
W.O.R.D.S
Words that are developmentally* appropriate (See
Handout 2)
Opportunities to use words in a meaningful way across
time and place, beginning with the immediate time and place and expanding to
other times and places
Representing/recording words in a form that encourages
remembering and retrieving
Dialogue is at the heart of word learning (Books and
Conversation)
Stories are the starting place!
|
Powerful
Vocabulary, NAEYC Conference, October 2016, Fran Toomey (toomeyand12@gmail.com} Handout 2: CHOOSING WORDS
Small Kids, Big Words
Laura Pappano Harvard, 2008 PreK-Gr 3
|
Creative Curriculum
Steps Before The Study:
https://www2.teachingstrategies.com/
|
Choosing a Topic For Learning
|
OWL: Making Every Word Count
(Page 27)
See Word Lists (pp 302-341)
|
Labels:
Development,
Early Literacy,
Oral Language,
Preschool,
Vocabulary
Monday, March 27, 2017
Vocabulary Development: 5A
Here are several short videos on how to help young children develop vocabulary:
Language and Learning: Infants and Toddlers
Uploaded on Feb 4, 2011
The purpose of this
video is to illustrate the role of the teacher in using language to support
children's development. The interactions you will see include positive language,
expansion, questioning, and redirection. Teachers use these techniques to
expand children's knowledge,
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7DPhIQh91Mw 14 minutes
Language Development Videos
Supporting Oral Language Development in a
Language-Rich Environment 9 minutes Ages
3-5
Published on Apr 13, 2016
Oral language is the
foundation for learning to read and write, and is critical for supporting the
development of children’s early literacy skills. This video introduces some of
the rule systems of language that children need to master to develop strong
oral language skills, and stresses
Scaffolding Language Skills
Vocabulary Development Words: Semantic, Syntactic, Phonology *Introduce words in context
Monday, March 20, 2017
Vocabulary: Part 4: Content Words in Story and Information Text
Tier 3: Content Words
My favorite source on vocabulary knowledge: K through HS is
the Text Project. There is an extensive
list of resources on this site. Here are
some that address Tier 3: Content Words for younger children.
Transportation
Clothes
Flight
Science (on the playground)
Critical Science Vocabulary
New words in science lessons and texts usually represent unknown
concepts (e.g., photosynthesis, erosion). This is different than the new
vocabulary in narratives that typically pertain to known concepts (e.g., new
words such as raucous and boisterous instead of the familiar noisy). In this
presentation, Elfrieda H. Hiebert [presenter] will discuss the need to make
science vocabulary education a priority in grades one through four. If the
concepts are not taught in grades one through four, later when the students are
in the middle grades they will be faced with an inordinate number of
challenging words, representing complex and unknown concepts
Labels:
Achievement Gap,
Early Literacy,
Oral Language,
Vocabulary
Monday, March 13, 2017
Vocabulary Part 3 Tiered Words
Top of Form
Bottom of Form
Excerpted from one of my other blogs: Dialogue About Language, Literacy, and Learning
Several distinctions have
been made about types of vocabulary words to be learned. One currently
popular distinction is categorizing “Tiered Words.” Beck formulated a system where she described
words as Tier 1, Tier 2 or Tier 3. Here’s a link to her work with Mc Keown and
Kucan (Creating Robust Vocabulary, Guildford Press, 2008):
al. al. describe these levels (tiers) as
Tier One: Most basic words,
rarely require instruction in school; Exs: clock, baby, happy
Tier Two: Words that are of high
frequency in mature language use and are found across a variety of domains;
Exs: coincidence, absurd, industrious.
Tier Three: Words whose frequency
of use is quite low and is often limited to specific domains; best learned when
needed in a content area; Exs: isotope, lathe, peninsula
(Note: Tiered vocabulary
is not to be confused with Tier 1, 2, and 3 Instruction in Response to
Intervention literature. Those tiers refer to the level of intervention needed
for students who are or are not progressing as expected in the regular
classroom. The instruction is not specific to vocabulary).
* * * *
A description of tiered words from an elementary school.
Taken from Images of Academic Words where there are many other
images/links. Simply google “images for academic words.”
****
And here is a commercial
site that provides a 3 minute video distinguishing tiers of words: http://www.learninga-z.com/commoncore/academic-vocabulary.html
Tier 1 words: These words are
basic vocabulary or the more common words most children will know. They include
high-frequency words and usually are not multiple meaning words.
Tier 2 words: Less familiar, yet
useful vocabulary found in written text and shared between the teacher and
student in conversation. The Common Core State Standards refers to these as “general
academic words.” Sometimes they are referred to as “rich vocabulary.” These
words are more precise or subtle forms of familiar words and include multiple
meaning words. Instead of walk for example, saunter could be used. These words
are found across a variety of domains.
Tier 3 words: CCSS refers to
these words as “domain specific;” they are critical to understanding the
concepts of the content taught in schools. Generally, they have low frequency use
and are limited to specific knowledge domains. Examples would include words
such as isotope, peninsula, refinery. They are best learned when teaching
specific content lessons, and tend to be more common in informational text.
So, we might consider Tier
2 words as general “academic words” while Tier 3 words are considered “content
specific words” referring to words/concepts learned in content areas like
science and social studies.
We can also distinguish
words important for instruction: frequency, conceptual complexity
(abstractness), and word relationships: familiarity, morphological family,
semantic relationships and dispersion.
One of the very best
sources on a range of vocabulary topics is the Text Project. See, for example:
Thursday, March 9, 2017
Vocabulary Development and Learning: Part 2B
The topic of vocabulary development is important enough to add some new and recent links/excerpts:
Recent Trends in Income, Racial, and Ethnic School
Readiness Gaps at Kindergarten Entry
Sean F.
Reardon, Ximena A. Portilla (August 2016 ). Recent Trends in Income, Racial,
and Ethnic School Readiness Gaps at Kindergarten Entry, American
Educational Research Association (AERA) Open, DOI:
10.1177/2332858416657343,
From
Reading Rockets
A short
excerpt:
“This study found that low-income kindergarten
students have reversed the trend of growing academic achievement gaps between
them and their higher-income peers. Academic achievement gaps grew from the
1970s to the 1990s, but from 1998 to 2010 the gaps shrank 10-16%. During this
time frame, the White-Hispanic kindergarten readiness gap and the White-Black
gap each dropped. Researchers attributed the improved preparedness, in part, to
low-income parents spending significantly more time reading to their children,
taking them to museums, and introducing them to educational games on computers.
Despite the narrowing of these readiness gaps, they remain large and, in fact, progress
is so slow that at the rate that improvements are occurring, it will take at
least 60 years for disparities to be eliminated.”
Teaching Vocabulary Early, Direct, and Sequential
By Andrew Biemiller
http://www.aft.org/periodical/american-educator/spring-2001/teaching-vocabulary#sthash.D6BkLdow.dpuf
A short excerpt
“Schools now do little to promote vocabulary development,
particularly in the critical years before grade 3. The role of schooling in
vocabulary acquisition has been the subject of much debate. Early (pre-literacy)
differences in vocabulary growth are associated with social class (Duncan,
Brooks-Gunn, and Klebanov, 1994; Hart and Risley, 1995; McLloyd, 1998). Nagy
and Herman (1987) and Sternberg (1987) argue that much vocabulary acquisition
results from literacy and wide reading rather than from direct instruction.
However, it is obvious that a great deal of vocabulary acquisition occurs
before children become literate, and before they are reading books that
introduce unfamiliar vocabulary (Becker, 1977). Cantalini (1987) and Morrison,
Williams, and Massetti (1998) both report that vocabulary acquisition in
kindergarten and grade 1 is little influenced by school experience, based on
finding that young first-graders have about the same vocabulary (Peabody
Picture Vocabulary Test) as older kindergarten children. Cantalini reported the
same result for second grade. - See more at: http://www.aft.org/periodical/american-educator/spring-2001/teaching-vocabulary#sthash.D6BkLdow.dpuf “
Closing the Vocabulary Gap
Alex Rappaport argues that word acquisition may be the easiest way to close the
achievement gap
A short excerpt:
“One
promise of public education is to level the playing field across the
socioeconomic and ethnic spectrum. Unfortunately, the system is not fulfilling
that promise. The achievement gap has been an issue for decades, and it’s
getting worse."
"A recent study released
by Stanford University sociologist Sean F. Reardon shows that the gap has
widened by 40% since the 1960s. The study looked at the disparity in academic
achievement between students in the tenth percentile of family income against
students in the ninetieth percentile. Standardized test scores were used as a
metric, which is fairly common in achievement gap studies. Other metrics
include high school dropout rates and college graduation rates. Unfortunately,
the relationship between income and achievement is consistent across all of
these metrics. According to Teach for America, only 8% of students growing up
in poverty graduate from college by age 24, compared with 80% of students in
more affluent areas. In other words, the effects of the gap extend beyond test
scores and make a significant impact on achievement throughout a student’s
academic career.”
"Many researchers attribute the lower achievement to “opportunity
gaps” such as a lack of educational resources at home, limited access to
health-care, and even more subtle factors like test bias, stereotyping, and
peer pressure. With so many social and cultural factors at play, the problem
can seem insurmountable. What to fix first? And how? Elimination of poverty or
improvements to the health care system can’t be achieved from within the
classroom walls" ….
Preschool
Through Third Grade Alignment and Differentiated Instruction: A Literature
Review
August 2016
Prepared for: Policy and Program Studies Service Office of Planning, Evaluation
and Policy Development U.S. Department of Education
Prepared by: Katie Drummond Aleksandra Holod
Marie Perrot Antonia Wang American Institutes for Research Washington, DC 20007
A short
excerpt from a 108 page report
“Executive
Summary This literature review provides a review of policies, programs, and
practices that have the potential to help students sustain the positive effects
of preschool as they progress from kindergarten through grade 3 (K–3). The U.S.
Department of Education’s Policy and Program Studies Service commissioned this
systematic literature review, which focuses on two specific approaches: (1)
preschool and K–3 alignment, and (2) differentiated instruction in kindergarten
and first grade. Background Research shows that participation in a high-quality
preschool can improve young children’s readiness skills for elementary school,
positively influencing behavioral, social-emotional, and cognitive outcomes
(Andrews, Jargowsky, & Kuhne, 2012). Specifically, for children who may be
at risk for academic challenges in early elementary school, attending a
high-quality preschool can improve test scores and attendance, and it can
reduce grade-level retention and placement in special education (Andrews et
al., 2012; Barnett, 2008; Karoly & Bigelow, 2005; Reynolds, 1993; Reynolds
et al., 2007). However, some preschool program evaluations document that strong
initial benefits may not persist into early elementary school (Lipsey, Farran,
& Hofer, 2015; Magnuson, Meyers, Ruhm, & Waldfogel, 2005; Manship,
Madsen, Mezzanotte, & Fain, 2013; Ramey et al., 2000; U.S. Department of
Health and Human Services, 2010). Preschool benefits may not persist for many
reasons, including lack of continuous follow-up with participating students,
lack of family supports or involvement, or limited intensity or duration of the
preschool program (Brooks-Gunn, 2003; Halpern, 2013; Reynolds, Magnuson, &
Ou, 2006). The positive effects of preschool may not persist if children attend
poor-quality elementary schools after preschool (Clements, Reynolds, &
Hickey, 2004; Lee & Loeb, 1995). Without additional and continuous supports
as children continue through the early elementary grades, participation in
preschool cannot overcome potential challenges that children, particularly
those at risk for poorer academic outcomes, may face. It is important to
identify ways to sustain early cognitive, social-emotional, and academic gains
in order to give all students opportunities to thrive academically. To explore
potential ways to sustain the positive effects of preschool, this literature
review focused on two specific topics: (1) preschool and K–3 alignment and (2)
differentiated instruction in kindergarten and first grade. The U.S. Department
of Education’s Policy and Program Studies Service (PPSS), in collaboration with
the Office of Early Learning, selected eight topics for preliminary searches
after initial attempts to identify interventions specifically designed to
sustain the benefits of preschool turned up low yields. Based on the search
results (and after receiving input from multiple Department offices), PPSS
recommended two final topics for the literature review. PPSS made final
decisions about further specifications for the differentiated instruction section
(e.g., only include research spanning grades K–1 and exclude studies that focus
exclusively on lower-achieving students)."
Preschool and K–3 Alignment The first topic
focuses on approaches to align preschool and kindergarten through grade 3.
"The first
topic focuses on approaches to align preschool and kindergarten through grade
3. Preschool or prekindergarten and K–3 alignment (sometimes called P–3)
emphasizes coordination among standards, curricula, instructional practices,
student assessment, and teacher professional development between the preschool
years and the early elementary school years. Early childhood experts assert
that the effects of preschool may be sustained and investment in early
education capitalized upon if curricula and instructional strategies from
preschool through grade 3 are well aligned (Bogard & Takanishi, 2005;
Brooks-Gunn, 2003; Howard, 2008). As Reynolds and Temple (2008) suggest, P–3
programs may provide more continuity and better organization of services for
students as well as enhanced school-family partnerships.
Differentiated
Instruction.
The second topic focuses on differentiated
instruction in kindergarten and first grade. The premise of differentiated
instruction is that teaching practices and curricula should vary to meet the
diverse needs and skills of the individual student and to optimize students’
learning experiences (Tomlinson, 2000, 2001). In a differentiated instructional
delivery model, student needs are emphasized (Stanford & Reeves, 2009),
with teachers purposively adapting instructional strategies and the focus of
skill building to be responsive to individual or groups of students (Jones,
Yssel, & Grant, 2012). One explanation for why initial benefits of
preschool do not persist as students enter elementary school is that children
who make early gains in preschool may not have the opportunity to maintain
their growth rate or learning trajectory because early elementary instruction
may focus on students who are less prepared and have low-level skills. In other
words, instruction may not be differentiated, and in some cases may not be
rigorous enough, to meet and build upon the skills that some students have upon
school entry (Claessens, Engel, & Curran, 2013; Kauerz, 2006; Lipsey,
Farran, & Hofer, 2015)."
Labels:
Achievement Gap,
Early Literacy,
Teachers,
Vocabulary
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)